Upgrade to Pro — share decks privately, control downloads, hide ads and more …

A metrical approach to ternarity in Northern Eu...

Pavel Iosad
May 27, 2023
83

A metrical approach to ternarity in Northern European accentual contrasts

Presentation at the 30th Manchester Phonology Meeting

Pavel Iosad

May 27, 2023
Tweet

Transcript

  1. Central claim • Many northern European contrasts between ‘accent 1’

    and ‘accent 2’ are three-way • ‘Too small’ • ‘Big enough, unmarked accent’ • ‘Big enough, marked accent’ • A serviceable analysis often equates ‘marked accent’ with ‘non-default foot construction’ 2
  2. Accentual systems in Northern Europe Ebenso bilden die Sprachen des

    Baltikums einen Sprachbund, den die Polytonie kennzeichnet; hierher gehören: das Schwedische, das Norwegische […], die meisten dänischen Dialekte, einige nord- deutsche Mundarten, das Nordkaschubische, das Litauische und Lettische, das Livische und Estnische. In den meisten Sprachen und Mundarten dieses Bundes ist die Tonverlaufkorrelation und in den übrigen ihre Abänderung, die Tonbruchkorrelation, vorhan- den. (Jakobson 1931) • To this we must add Franconian (West Germanic) and Scottish Gaelic (Ternes 1980) 3
  3. North Germanic • ‘Accent 1’ vs. ‘accent 2’: Norwegian ¹badet

    ‘bath-DEF’ ≠ ²bade ‘bathe’ • Neutralized to accent 1 in monosyllables: ¹bad ‘bath’ 4
  4. West Germanic: Franconian • Contrast in final and non-final syllables:

    • Mayen ¹šāl ‘scarf’ ≠ ²šāl ‘stale’ • Mayen ¹šal ‘cover’ ≠ ²šal ‘sound’ • Maastricht ¹spøːlə ‘rinse’ ≠ ²spøːlə ‘play’ • Usually no contrast in CV([obstruent]) stressed syllables • Mayen ˈzat ‘sated, full’ vs. ¹šāt ‘pity’ vs. ²štāt ‘stand.PRS.3SG’ 5
  5. West Germanic: Low German • Usually final syllables, MLG long

    vowels • ¹vīt ‘wide’ ≠ ²vīd̥ /²vīt ‘willow’ • But no contrast in vɪt ‘white’ • Realizations/correlates unclear/various • Tone/prosody (‘accent’)? • Length vs. overlength? • Vowel quality [vɪt] ‘white’ vs. [viː(ː)t] ‘wide’/‘willow’ 6
  6. Gaelic • Generally final/penultimate syllables • Historical long vowels/diphtongs vs.

    disyllables with hiatus: ¹bogha ‘bow’ ≠ ²bò ‘cow’ • Historical monosyllables with excrescent vowel vs. disyllables: ballag ‘skull’ [¹paɫak] ≠ balg ‘belly’ [²paɫak] • No contrast in light monosyllables: math ‘good’ 7
  7. Baltic • Lithuanian: ‘acute’ vs. ‘circumflex’ • šáukite ‘shoot-IMP.2PL’ vs.

    šaũkite ‘call-IMP.2PL’ • Latvian: three accents in main-stressed syllables • mī̃t ‘exchange.INF’ ≠ mī̀t ‘live.PRS.3SG’ ≠ mī̂t ‘tread.INF’ • Accent contrasts only in CVː or CV[sonorant] syllables • Lithuanian lùpa ‘peel.PRS.3SG’ (‘stress’) vs. lū́pa ‘lip’ (‘acute accent’) vs. lū̃žis ‘break’ (‘circumflex accent’) 8
  8. Danish • Stød, a laryngealization prosody • Contrast in both

    final and non-final syllables: • kol ‘coal’ ≠ kolˀd ‘cold’, team ‘team’ ≠ liˀm ‘glue’ • biˀbel ‘bible’ ≠ engel ‘angel’ • No stød possible in CV([obstruent]) syllables 9
  9. Livonian • Stød contrasts in CVV and CV[voice] syllables •

    ūdõ ‘cook.INF’ ≠ ū’dõ ‘strain.INF’ • vannõ ‘swear.INF’ ≠ va’nnõ ‘old.PART.SG’ • No stød in light syllables: va(*’)dā ‘fishing net’ but PART.SG va’ddõ 10
  10. First primary claim Language Accent 1 Accent 2 Rhine Franconian

    ¹šāt ²štāt Norwegian ¹badet ²bade Danish kol kolˀd Lithuanian lū́ pa lū̃žis Low German ¹vit ²vit viːt viːːt Lewis Gaelic ¹bogha ²bò Argyll Gaelic boˀgha bò Livonian ūdõ ū’dõ 11
  11. First primary claim Language No basis Accent 1 Accent 2

    Rhine Franconian ˈzat ¹šāt ²štāt Norwegian ¹bad ¹badet ²bade Danish kat kol kolˀd Lithuanian lùpa lū́ pa lū̃žis Low German vɪt ¹vit ²vit vit viːt viːːt Lewis Gaelic (¹)math ¹bogha ²bò Argyll Gaelic math boˀgha bò Livonian vadā ūdõ ū’dõ 11
  12. Sonority-defined basis • In many cases, basis is a ‘heavy

    sonority’ rhyme: • Long vowel • or sonorant/voiced coda • Examples are • Danish stød • (Rhine) Franconian • Baltic • Livonian (voiced coda) • Norwegian: Flekkefjord, Salten (Lorentz 2008) 12
  13. Size-defined basis • Contrast only within domains of a certain

    size • Norwegian/Swedish: contrast in disyllabic domain, accent 1 when there is no following syllable • North Germanic ‘circumflex accent’: å kâst ‘throw-INF’ vs. et ¹kast ‘throw’ • Circumflex monosyllables are superheavy (Kristoffersen 2011) ⇒ trimoraic basis • East Slesvig Danish (Bjerrum 1949), with apparent contrast in CV syllables: ¹hy ‘high’ ≠ ²hy ‘height’ • In fact, accent 2 adds a mora (Goldshtein 2021), so ²hy is an accentual minimal pair with ¹tō ‘two’ ⇒ bimoraic basis 13
  14. Special case: Scottish Gaelic • CV syllables do not support

    the accent contrast • Accent 1 (?) in ‘tonal’ dialects • Literature claims that /CV/ syllables surface as [CVˀ] due to stress-to-weight, but this is doubtful (Morrison 2019) • Rather: glottalization is predictable in monomoraic domains • Sandhi glottalization: gheobh [jo] ‘receive.FUT’ but gheobh i [joˀ i] ‘she will receive’ • Bimoraic basis 14
  15. The fundamental generalization • The basis for accentual contrasts is

    size • Syllables lacking basis are, in some relevant sense, too small • Ternarity arises because syllables lacking basis are incompatible with the marked accent 15
  16. Ternary quantity: Estonian • Minimal triplets • lina ‘flax.GEN/PART’ ≠

    linna ‘town.GEN’ ≠ ˋlinna ‘town.PART’ • sagi ‘bustle.IMP.2SG’ ≠ saagi ‘yield.GEN’ ≠ ˋsaagi ‘saw.PART’ • Well established generalizations: • Light syllables are Q1 • Heavy syllables are Q2 or Q3 • Q2 syllables must be followed by another syllable • Q3 is lexically and morphologically conditioned 16
  17. Estonian gradation Nucleus C Q1 Q2 Q3 Short N talu

    [l] ˋtallu [lːl] ‘farm.GEN/PART’ ‘farm.ILL’ NN kulli [lː] ˋkulli [lːl] ‘hawk.GEN’ ‘hawk.PART/ILL’ T saba [p] sappa [pːp] ‘tail.GEN/PART’ ‘tail.ILL’ TT lõpu [pː] lõppu [pːp] ‘end.GEN’ ‘end.PART/ILL’ Long N piima [iː] ˋpiima [iːi] ‘milk.GEN’ ‘milk.PART/ILL’ T koodi [oː] ˋkoodi [oːo] ‘code.GEN’ ‘code.PART/ILL’ TT riigi [iːk] riiki [iːkːk] ‘state.GEN’ ‘state.PART/ILL’ 17
  18. Second principal claim • Basis restrictions are ultimately interpretable in

    terms of structural size • This follows if the marked accent is a particular kind of metrical structure • Syllables lack basis when they lack the segmental substrate to build that kind of metrical structure 18
  19. Corollary: the use of neutralization criteria • In Norwegian/Swedish, accent

    2 can be considered marked because monosyllables always have accent 1 (Riad 2009) • Default assignment ≫ lack of basis • Estonian: • Q1 = (LL), Q2 = (HL), default in disyllables, Q3 = (H) • It follows, correctly, that H monosyllables are automatically Q3: strong grade Q1jõgi ‘river’, weak grade Q3jõe ‘river.GEN’ Wd Ft σ ¹bɑːd Wd Ft σ ²bɑː σ də Wd Ft σ ¹bɑː də Wd Ft σ Q1li σ nɑ Wd Ft σ Q2lin σ nɑ Wd Ft σ Q3lin nɑ 19
  20. Corollary: taking the tone out of tonal accents • No

    commitment to tone as the mechanism implementing contrast • Direct account of cue variability • Livonian stød • Dialect variation in cues (or rather cue weighting): Gaelic • Diachronic permeability • Quantity > pitch (Estonian, Low German?) • Pitch > laryngealization (North Germanic, Gaelic, Livonian, Leivu Estonian) • Pitch > quantity (Gaelic?) • Laryngealization > pitch (Baltic?) 20
  21. Corollary: ternarity is not a given i • Low German

    (Kohler 2001): • Q1 / short V / no basis = lax [ʋɪt] ‘white’ • Q2 / long V / basis = tense short [ʋit] ‘wide’ • Q3 / overlong V / accent = tense long [ʋiːt] ‘willow’ Lax Tense Language Short Long Short Long English, German ɪ i(ː) Dutch ɪ i Scots ɪ i iː North Low Saxon ɪ ¹i(ː) ²iː(ː) West Frisian ɪ i iː Icelandic ɪ ɪː i iː 21
  22. Corollary: ternarity is not a given ii • When basis

    is quantitative, adding quantity to the smallest syllable type makes it long and creates basis ⇒ ternarity • With qualitative basis, adding quantity can fill the gap without creating accentual basis • East Frisian Low German (Lücht 2013): • Tense length: [ziː(ː)t] ‘silk’ < *ziːdə • Lax length: [ruːəp] ‘seal’ < *rʊbə 22
  23. Conclusions • Most Northern European accent systems submit to the

    ternary analysis • Fundamentally, basis is about size • Metrical approaches to accentual systems capture this directly • See also Pöchtrager (2023, and passim) on vowel quality as size • The metrical approach gives the right level of abstraction • Cue variability • If the analytical key is tone (or laryngealization), does the link with size follow? 23
  24. Questions and prospects • Empirical work on variation remains outstanding

    • More-than-ternary systems: tone after all? • Standard Latvian (Krämer) • Low Latvian, Lithuanian dialects • Diachronic and areal links, including the life cycle • Stretch target: a fully privative analysis of markedness 24